Phonetics

An analysis of listening errors by Korean EFL learners from self-paced passage dictation

Hyesun Cho 1 , *
Author Information & Copyright
1Department of Education, Dankook University, Yongin, Korea
*Corresponding author : hscho@dankook.ac.kr

© Copyright 2021 Korean Society of Speech Sciences. This is an Open-Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Received: Jan 31, 2021; Revised: Mar 11, 2021; Accepted: Mar 13, 2021

Published Online: Mar 31, 2021

Abstract

In this study, listening errors by Korean EFL learners are comprehensively analyzed from self-paced passage dictation tasks. Fifty-five Korean EFL learners participated in the study. Listeners were asked to write down dictation passages as accurately as possible, while listening to the audio as much as they needed. The results show that (i) low-proficiency learners tend to misperceive longer phrases than high-proficiency learners, (ii) function words are more often omitted or misheard than content words, and (iii) low-proficiency learners have more difficulties with content words than high-proficiency learners do. Most frequent suffix errors were omissions of past or plural suffixes. Among the function words, the most frequent errors were found with auxiliary contractions, infinitive marker to, and articles, mostly in the environment of linking and elision. It is also shown that C-V linking, C-C linking, and elision are the primary sources for the most frequent errors. C-V linking led to errors in correctly locating the word boundary, while C-C linking and elision resulted in omission. These errors show that Korean EFL listeners have difficulties in detecting fine-grained phonetic details to the extent that native speakers can do.

Keywords: L2 listening error; function words; content words; linking; elision

1. Introduction

1.1. Difficulties in L2 listening

This paper examines Korean EFL listeners' phonetic difficulties based on self-paced passage dictation tasks. Listening is considered a difficult skill to learn, because underlying speech sounds are modified in connected speech through various phonological processes such as linking, reduction and elision (Johnson, 2004). Due to the differences between spoken and written language, English learners often mishear what they can understand in written forms, displaying a gap between listening and reading proficiencies. The purpose of this study is to provide a quantitative and comprehensive analysis of L2 listeners' listening difficulties in single vs. multi words and in different word categories, content vs. function words.

Hwang (2004) studied listening difficulties with two different levels of text and English proficiency in high school students. Students listened to a spoken text, verbally described what they heard, and then reflected on and wrote down any difficulties they encountered in listening. She found that listening difficulties predominantly involved non-recognition of sequences of words.

That is, listeners identified only a few words from a long phrase. For example, only leg is heard in “.. its short legs and clumsy looking”. The error rate in word-sequence recognition was significantly higher in less proficient learners than more proficient learners. Low-proficiency learners also showed higher error rate in mishearing a word (e.g., bat heard as back). Errors in phonetic decoding ('non-recognition') were more common than errors of other types, e.g., understanding meaning of a word that is correctly heard ('non-grasp') (p. 288). In other words, learners may miss the words that they can easily understand in written forms. Based on these results, she suggests that predominant listening difficulty is phonetic perception, so developing basic decoding skills is important (p. 294). Phonetically-accurate perception is the most important element in order to improve one's listening proficiency.

Choung (2014) also highlighted the difficulties and importance of aural perception practice in her study of high school students' listening strategy. Among various listening strategies, including metacognitive and affective ones, the students felt that phonetic perception practice (such as listening to British English and linked sounds) is the most effective and facilitate the use of other metacognitive strategies.

What makes spoken English harder to understand is connected-speech phenomena such as linking, elision, contraction, and reduced forms. Linking has been identified as a phonetic obstacle that hinders listening comprehension (Choung, 2014; Hagiwara & Kuzumaki, 1982; Jeong & Koo, 2017). Function words are usually monosyllables, so when they undergo linking (e.g., make a [meɪkə]), they can sound like a part of the preceding word. Moreover, function words easily undergo reduction and elision due to their lack of stress. Reduced vowels have lower intensity and shorter duration, which makes them particularly more difficult to perceive than content words. In Kang (2017), the subjects showed higher error rate in function words than in content words. In Hagiwara & Kuzumaki (1982), in dictation data by Japanese learners of English, high frequency errors involved function words: demonstrative that (in go to that restaurant) was misheard as the, auxiliary will was omitted, article a was omitted, and past suffixes were frequently omitted (e.g., listened to is misheard as listen to).

1.2. Error analysis using dictation

Dictation is a common way of assessing listening difficulties of L2 learners. In dictation tasks, students are asked to write down full sentences or fill in the blanks (Chang & Chang, 2014). Test sentences are usually played to the subjects a limited number of times. Researchers often pre-determine target phonological processes and include them in test words and phrases. For example, Kang (2017) constructed 7 sentences for dictation task, which are designed to include function words (preposition, pronoun, conjunction), played three times to the students. Jeong & Koo (2017) adapted 27 sentences from a TOEIC practice book containing various phonological processes that hinders aural perception, such as linking, assimilation, elision, and weak forms. Listeners showed highest error rates in vowel-vowel linking, progressive assimilation, and consonant-vowel linking.

Another way of collecting listening errors is through the observation of relatively larger-scale dictation, without hand-picking difficult sounds or phonological processes beforehand. Kim (2002) gave dictation assignments to graduate students of interpretation major with high-intermediate to advanced English proficiency. The materials were TV news broadcast of about 20 minutes, assigned each week. In the transcriptions, he observed many errors in function words, though quantitative information was not provided. Due to the process of contraction and reduction, function words are easily missed (They should have done this is transcribed as They _this), or misheard (to win the race is transcribed as the window racer a). His work focused more on illustrating errors in content words and phrases on the ground that difficulties of function words are evident. He classified the error phrases by word position (initial, mid, final in word), wrong syllabification (was hit misheard as with it), wrong separation (consider misheard as can see a), and fusing (wrongly combined words, to read misheard as tree). However, quantitative analyses are not given, and content words and function words are not fully separately analyzed.

Yang & Kang (2020) attempted to find out a full range of listening error types from a self-annotated transcription task that they developed. They used 50 short sentences (ten sentences×five assignments) selected from a TOEFL practice book. Students were asked to transcribe English sentences and then mark the parts where they had problems and describe them. From the transcription data by 19 students, they reported six error types: substitution (floral for floor), wrong segmentation (to wishin for tuition), wrong merge (ferries for fare), omission (absent-mind for absent-minded), insertion (days for day), spelling and blank (p. 41). However, while this way of classifying errors shows what happened to the difficult sounds, it does not directly reveal what are the sounds that are difficult to listen.

The present study also aims to discover learners' common errors in a full range, but unlike Yang & Kang (2020), classifies the error types based on length (one word or a word sequence) and type of words (function word or content word). In addition, instead of constructing sentences to contain some particular phonological processes, students are asked to transcribe a whole passage, so that an overall picture of difficulties can be captured. Most dictation tasks have been carried out at word or phrase level (fill-in-the-blanks) or sentence level. Research based on whole-phrase dictation by Korean EFL learners is not much found in the literature, except Kang (2017).

The transcription data collected from whole-passage dictation tasks may serve as a learner corpus. It is generally known that corpora allow research based on real-world data, rather than experimental settings. Learner corpora, consisting of L2 learner output, can contribute not only to study of characteristics of L2 learners' language, but also to development of a computer-assisted language learning system (CALL) (Kotani & Yoshimi, 2015). Listening corpora, consisting of EFL learners' response to spoken data, have been relatively rare (Kotani & Yoshimi, 2015). The L2 listening corpus created by Kotani & Yoshimi (2015) consists of two components, phonetic recognition and comprehension, including text data transcribed from a dictation exercise.

In the present study, students are allowed to play, pause, and repeat the assigned audio files as much as they want. This will help release psychological obstacles such as attention, working memory and anxiety problems (Choung, 2014; Hwang, 2004; Rubin, 1994; Yang & Kang, 2020) that can arise during listening to materials just once or a few limited number of times. This way we can tease apart phonetic difficulties from psychological obstacles, and identify phonetic problems separately. A drawback is that it is different from usual real-life listening or language-test situations (Voss, 1984).

This paper is organized as follows: Section 2 explains the research method. Section 3 presents the results describing listening errors in terms of the number of connected words, function vs. content words, suffix errors, and errors in word sequences. Section 4 is discussion and conclusion.

2. Research method

2.1. Subject

Subjects were 55 Korean students (26 male, 29 female) who enrolled in TOEIC Listening course in a university. Of these students, 39 students have taken a TOEIC test and their score average is 795 and the median is 749. This corresponds to the English proficiency level of intermediate to high intermediate.

2.2. Listening materials

Listening materials were four TOEIC passages, two conversations and two telephone messages, from a TOEIC textbook (ETS, 2018). The instructor assigns a passage for dictation each week for four weeks (total 4 passages). The details are given in Table 1.

Table 1. Description of passages (CT and FN are the number of content words and function words, respectively. WPM is word per minute.)
Passage Word count Dur (sec) WPM #Sentences Genre (Topic)
CT FN
1 118 38 186 12 Conversation (Performance review)
56 (47%) 62 (53%)
2 122 39 191 16 Conversation (Piano repair)
58 (48%) 64 (52%)
3 103 29 162 9 Telephone message (Missing paycheck)
51 (50%) 52 (50%)
4 90 28 142 10 Telephone message (Pharmacy prescription)
50 (56%) 40 (44%)
Total 433 134 171 (Mean) 47
215 (50%) 218 (50%)
Download Excel Table

The total word count is 432, consisting of 215 content words (50%) and 218 function words (50%). Mean duration of the audio files was 33.5 seconds. Contracted words were treated as two words (UCLES, 2021). In the fourth column, WPM is speech rate (word per minute) (mean of 171). In total, there were 433 words (47 sentences) in four passages. For each passage, 52, 51, 51, and 50 students submitted their dictation homework. This makes the total number of targeted words 22,111 (the number of students×word count for each passage) (10,971 content words, 11,140 function words) and the number of sentences 2,399 (the number of students× sentence count for each passage) to be analyzed.

In addition, among the content words, there were a total of 24 inflectional suffix instances (14 plural, 6 third-person singular, 4 regular past-tense suffixes) in the listening materials. Multiplied by the number of students, the total number of targeted suffixes is 1,230.

Text complexity was measured using the ATOS text analyzer1, which calculates the readability level for short text passages. The results are shown in Table 2.

Table 2. ext complexity of the listening passages
Passages 1 2 3 4 All
ATOS level 4.4 3.8 5.9 6.0 4.8
Average word length 4.4 4 4.5 4.4 4.3
Average sentence length 7.8 7.8 11.2 11 9.4
Average vocabulary level 2.43 2.6 3 2.9 2.9
Download Excel Table

According to this, ATOS readability level, average sentence length, and average vocabulary level were higher in passages 3 and 4 (telephone messages) than passages 1 and 2 (conversations). Average word length was similar in all passages.

2.3. Research procedure

Subjects were asked to transcribe a passage once a week for four consecutive weeks as homework. They were allowed to listen to the passage as much as they needed, and asked to write down the passage with a pencil as accurately as they can. They were instructed to stop the audio wherever they can write what they heard as a chunk. After completing dictation, they compared their own passage with the answer script given in their textbook and corrected errors using a color pen, and wrote down unknown vocabularies. After finishing the corrections, they took a picture of their passage and submit it through the online learning management system. Students were given participation points, but not evaluated by their performance in dictation. The researcher collected the submitted pictures and manually entered errors in an Excel spreadsheet. R Studio (version 1.3.959; RStudio Team, 2020) was used for statistical analyses.

The present research focuses on length of word sequences and types of words where errors occur. Thus, error words were first classified into the number of word sequences (single word, word sequences), and then single-word errors were further classified into content words, function words, and suffixes. Among the error types, I examined only substitution and omission errors. Insertion errors (e.g., back as backs) are not analyzed, because they account for only a small portion of the errors (5% of all the errors) (cf. the insertion error rate was 4.5% in Yang & Kang [2020]). Spelling errors, considered unrelated to listening, were ignored (cf. Hagiwara & Kuzumaki, 1982).

3. Results

3.1. Number of connected words

A total of 970 error words and word sequences were identified from students' passage dictation. Excluding 51 insertion errors, 919 errors were analyzed. This includes 710 single-word errors and 209 word-sequence errors. Single-word errors mean substitution or omission errors in a single word, where immediately adjacent words are correctly heard. Word-sequence errors mean a sequence of words where errors are found in all the connected words. For example, when “these sorts of” is misheard as “these all to”, it is considered as one word-sequence error with two (not three) connected error-words. In other words, the single word or word sequence criterion is the number of words where errors occur consecutively.

It can be expected that low-proficiency learners will have a greater difficulty in understanding longer phrases than single words. To examine for this, the number of connected error-words was compared with students' scores (mean of midterm and final exam test scores). Figure 1 shows the number of connected words plotted against the students' scores. R function jitter was used to avoid overplotting. A linear regression was performed with number of connected error-words as the independent variable, and scores as the dependent variable. As the number of connected words increases, students' scores tend to decrease significantly (t(917)=–2.29, p<0.05). This indicates that low proficiency learners tend to miss longer phrases than high proficiency learners. In particular, it can be noted that high-score students (above 80) are mostly found in the upper-left corner of the plot. This indicates that high-proficiency learners are most likely to miss just one word, or two subsequent words. This result conforms to Hwang (2004: 288) where low proficiency learners showed a higher error rate in word sequences than high proficiency learners.

pss-13-1-17-g1
Figure 1. Number of connected error-words and score
Download Original Figure
3.2. Single words
3.2.1. Content vs. function words

Single-word errors were classified further into errors in content words (CT), function words (FN), and suffixes (SF). Table 3 shows the frequency of errors in each category. The second column is the raw count of errors and the third column is the percentage of errors relative to the total number of single-word errors (710). The last column shows the percentage of errors relative to the number of all words or suffixes in each category. Figure 2 shows the errors relative to the total number of words in each class.

Table 3. Frequency of errors
Class Raw count % in total errors % in the category (total number)
Content words 172 24 1.6 (10,971)
Function words 471 66 4.2 (11,140)
Suffix 67 9 5.4 (1,230)
Download Excel Table
pss-13-1-17-g2
Figure 2. Errors by word/suffix categories CT, content words; FN, function words; SF, suffixes.
Download Original Figure

Table 3 and Figure 2 show that the error rate is higher in function words than content words, in both raw absolute and relative frequencies. This is a similar result to Kang (2017). In college students' listening tasks in Kang (2017), function words showed error rate of 50%–58%, compared to 25%–29% of content words. These rates are similar to the current result, but here function words show a higher error-rate (66%) and content words show a lower error-rate (24%) than Kang (2017), so a greater difference is found. The relative frequencies in the last column confirm this finding: function word errors are more frequent than content word words. Table 2 also shows that suffix errors account for 9% of the total errors. However, in terms of the relative frequencies (in the last column), suffix errors are the most frequent2.

Figure 3 shows score distribution in each category. Content words have a lower median (the vertical line) than function words. This means that content words posed more difficulties in lower-proficiency learners than in high-proficiency learners. This may indicate that low-proficiency learners have less vocabulary knowledge than high-proficiency learners. A linear regression was conducted with word class as the independent variable, and scores as the dependent variable. The results show that the difference is statistically significant. The scores in content vs. function words are significantly different from each other (t(916)=3.43, p<0.001). Score is significantly higher by 3.1 in function words. Content words and suffix errors are not significantly different from each other (t(916)=1.21, p=0.2).

pss-13-1-17-g3
Figure 3. Errors by word/suffix categories and scores CT, content words; FN, function words; SF, suffixes
Download Original Figure
3.2.2. Function words

Figure 4 and Table 4 show the raw count and relative frequency of subtypes of function words. In terms of the relative frequency, the three most frequent function word errors are found in Aux-Cont (contracted forms of auxiliaries and modals), infinitive marker to (in just to make sure), and articles.

pss-13-1-17-g4
Figure 4. Total (grey) and error (black) counts by subtypes of function words AUX-CONT, auxiliary contraction; ART, article; PREP, preposition; CONJ, conjunction; AUX, auxiliary; TO, infinitive marker to; POSS, possessive; DEM, demonstrative; PRO, pronoun; NEG, negative contraction
Download Original Figure
Table 4. The frequency of errors by subtypes of function words
FN subtype (a) Raw count % in total error (b) Subtype total % in subtype (=a/b)
Aux-Cont 118 25 1,076 11.0
To (inf) 24 5 253 9.5
ART 105 22 1,174 8.9
Neg-Cont 9 2 205 4.4
CONJ 39 8 921 4.2
PREP 88 19 2,298 3.8
POSS 23 5 714 3.2
AUX 32 7 1,123 2.8
DEM 19 4 612 2.0
PRO 14 3 2,660 0.8
Download Excel Table

Table 5 shows the most frequent function word errors with error rate of 12% and more. The fourth column illustrates examples of the most frequent error for a given word (the numbers in parentheses are frequency). The rates show the frequency of errors relative to each given word. The most frequent errors are found with auxiliary contractions, 's (has) and 're (are). The former was mostly omitted, and the latter, you're, was perceived as your. The frequent error words also include prepositions (under), possessive our, were with negative contraction (weren't). Wrong separation is found in under, which is misheard as on the (cf. Kim, 2002). The most common type of errors is omission. Misperception of the article a as the most frequently arises after with, where linking between with and a very likely makes a sound like the. Thus, linking is a crucial cause for listening difficulty.

Table 5. The most frequent function-word errors (The number in the curly brackets indicates omission errors)
FN subtype Word Freq. Error example Context
AUX-CONT ‘s (has) 53/206 (26%) {48} she’s been, nobody’s said, mistake’s been
AUX-CONT ’re (are) 13/51 (25%) your (9) understand you’re in
PREP under 11/51 (21%) on the (9) still under warranty
POSS our 8/51 (16%) {4} I (3) all our keyboard
AUX weren’t 8/51 (16%) won’t (3) that weren’t included
ART a 63/459 (14%) {24} the (19) with a one year
DEM these 7/51 (14%) this (4) These sorts of
CONJ and 12/102 (12%) n (8) (e.g., Kenten) Kent and Tracey
AUX could 6/51 (12%) {4} If you could contact
CONJ that 18/154 (12%) {11} possible that your / month that weren’t / know that your
Download Excel Table
3.2.3. Content words

There were 10 content words that have error frequency of 5 or more, as shown in Table 6.

Table 6. The most frequent content-word errors
Word Freq. Error example Context
evaluation 23/52 (44%) devaluation (19) first evaluation.
had 10/52 (19%) have (9) I had my
away 10/52 (19%) a way (6) been away on
though 7 /50(14%) {6} tomorrow, though.
shipment 5/50 (10%) ship (4) new shipment in
issues 5/51 (10%) {3} payroll issues.
shifts 5/51 (10%) ships (2) weekend shifts last
weekend 5/51 (10%) we can (2) extra weekend shifts
division 5/52 (10%) business (2) sales division for
Download Excel Table

Of these, the most frequent error is found with evaluation, which is misheard as devaluation. The first vowel of evaluation is linked with the final sound of the preceding word first, resulting in misperception. There was no audible pause between first and evaluation, so [t] in first and [ɪ] in evaluation are linked and sounds like unstressed [tɪ], which is likely heard as [dɪ]. As for had, it is unclear what causes the error (have), since usually [d] and [v] are not considered confusable. It could be the influence of the following labial sound [m]. Wrong separation errors (Kim, 2002; Yang & Kang, 2020) are found with away (misheard as a way) and weekend (misheard as we can). Shipment is often misheard as ship. Only the stressed syllable ship is perceived, whereas the unstressed -ment [mn̩], with syllabic consonant and [t] deletion, is omitted. Misperception of [f] as [p], which are not contrastive in Korean, is found in the error of shifts as ships.

3.2.4. Suffixes

Suffix errors account for 9% of the single word errors. Table 7 shows the frequency of each type of the suffix errors. The most frequent errors are found with past suffix -ed, followed by plural suffix -s. The 3rd person singular -s is omitted only 3 times of 308. Nearly all suffix-errors were omission errors (65/67=97%).

Table 7. The frequency of types of suffixes
Type Frequency Error example Context
Past 17/205 (8%) work (15) I worked three
Plural 45/717 (6%) month (21)
shift (15)
several months before weekend shifts last
3rdSg 3/308 (1%) get (2) she gets back
Download Excel Table
3.3. Word sequences

Table 8 shows word-sequence errors that occurred with frequency of 6 or more errors. These errors take up 39% of the total word-sequence errors (81/209).

Table 8. The frequency of word sequence error (The number in the curly brackets indicates omission errors.)
Word sequence Freq. Error example Context
You’ve received 18/51 (35%) you receive (10) sure __ this
Payroll issues 11/51 (22%) pay rolly shoes (2) charge of __.
That weren’t 11/51 (22%) {6} month __ included in
A mistake’s 10/51 (20%) mistakes (7) I think _ been made
These sorts of 9/51 (18%) this source (2) definitely! __ repairs
Called in 8/50 (16%) {3} calling (2) your doctor __ your
Away on 8/52 (15%) a when (3) she’s been __ business
That weren’t included in 6/51 (12%) {2} month __ my paycheck
Download Excel Table

The most frequent error-sequences include auxiliary contraction (you've received, a mistake's) and negative contraction (weren't). Others involve C-V linking (payroll issues, called _in, that weren't included _in) and elision due to C-C linking (these _sorts of). A similar error involving C-V linking has been reported in Japanese learners of English (interest _ininteresting) (Hagiwara & Kuzumaki, 1982: 57). Wrong segmentation is found in away on, misperceived as a when (cf. Kim, 2002; Yang & Kang, 2020).

3.4. Misperception of linking and elision

As shown in the results above, it is clear that linking and elision make L2 listening harder in connected speech. Words and word sequences involving linking and elision were often misheard or omitted. L2 listeners had difficulties in correctly perceiving linked word-sequences, such as first evaluation, payroll issues. Figure 5 shows spectrograms of first evaluation, where the final consonant in the preceding word is linked with the initial vowel in the subsequent word. As can be seen in the spectrograms, there seem to be no salient cues to boundary between the two words. It thus can be difficult for L2 learners to decide whether the [t] belongs to the first or second word.

pss-13-1-17-g5
Figure 5. Spectrogram of first evaluation
Download Original Figure

In particular, the most frequent error substitute for evaluation was devaluation (Table 6). In Figure 5, the stop closure for [t] in first is short, immediately followed by the next vowel without a pause, so it can be ambiguous to the L2 listeners. Moreover, short release duration may lead to perception of [d] instead of [t]. Whereas it is known that native speakers successfully utilize the juncture, or word boundary, information (e.g., VOT; Altenberg, 2005), the Korean EFL learners were not able to use the juncture cues and made errors in correctly locating the boundary.

This substitution cannot come from the top-down process, because evaluation would be better than devaluation in terms of the context (It was three monthsbefore I had my first devaluation), and in terms of frequency (the COCA frequency of first evaluation is 36, first devaluation is 1). Considering this, in the substitution error of evaluation as devaluation, listeners seemed to use bottom-up information (acoustic signal) rather than top-down, contextual, information. However, the lack of sensitivity to fine-grained juncture cues led to the wrong-segmentation errors.

The same applies to other phrases involving C-V linking. As in first evaluation, errors involving C-V linking result in wrong segmentation, a failure to correctly locating word boundary. Some subjects misperceived payroll issues as pay rollyshoes, as if the [ɪ] in the following word issues belongs to the preceding word. Accordingly, issues were perceived as shoes, an error in correctly locating the word boundary.

Misperception of C-C linked phrases was also common, particularly with auxiliary contractions, such as nobody's said. Frication interval is longer in double than single fricatives, but Korean EFL learners have difficulties in distinguishing between single vs. geminate fricatives across word-boundary (Shin & Hwang, 2012). L2 listeners are less sensitive to this kind of fine-grained phonetic differences than native speakers (Best, 1995; Desmerules-Trudel, 2018).

4. Discussion and Conclusion

To summarize the results, low proficiency learners tend to have more errors in longer sequences. They also tend to misperceive content words more often than higher proficiency learners do. Function words showed more errors than content words. Among the function words, the most frequent errors were auxiliary contraction, infinitive marker to, and articles. Auxiliary contraction (you've, nobody's), articles (a, the), prepositions (in), are linked with the adjacent word or elided, making them harder to perceive. The most frequent suffix errors were omission of past suffixes (workedwork). Content-word errors mostly come from linking (first evaluationfirst devaluation), confusing similar sounds (shiftsship), and possibly from the reduction of unstressed syllable (shipmentship).

Word-sequence errors show consecutive combination of these errors: auxiliary contraction and suffix omission (you've receivedyou receive), wrong segmentation due to linking (payroll issuespay rolly shoes), omission of conjunction, auxiliary, and negative contraction (that weren't → ø).

Linking is a frequent source of errors. C-V linking results in wrong segmentation, and C-C linking results in omission of final sounds of the preceding words, which are often auxiliary contractions. The results reaffirm that, due to these difficulties in phonetic decoding, listening for reduction and juncture should be explicitly taught (Celce-Murcia et al., 2010).

In addition, L2 learners would have to learn how to utilize fine-grained phonetic cues that may help them detect the juncture (in C-V linking) and distinguish single vs. double consonants (in C-C linking and elision). Even if they explicitly learned linking and elision rules, there would not be acoustic cues enough for L2 learners to detect. For accurate perception, learners will have to develop sensitivity to fine-grained phonetic details, such as the durational difference between single vs. double consonants. This is an achievable goal, since literature show that advanced L2 learners can detect fine-grained phonetic details to the level comparable to native speakers (Shoemaker, 2010; Zhang & Wang, 2019).

To conclude, the current study showed what aspects of phonetic perception are the most difficult for Korean EFL learners. It is reconfirmed that connected speech phenomena such as linking and elision are the primary source for phonetic difficulties, for both function words and content words. Improvements can be made if they learn the linking and elisions rules, and sensitivity to the fine-grained phonetic details in the linked or elided sounds.

Notes

2 An anonymous reviewer raised a possibility that suffix omission can be a spelling error, which is often found in EFL writing as well. Since there is no way of telling spelling errors from listening errors for suffixes in the dictation transcriptions, I analyzed the data as is written by the participants.

References

1.

Altenberg, E. P. (2005). The perception of word boundaries in a second language. Second Language Research, 21(4), 325-358.

2.

Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realistic view of cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: issues in cross-language research (pp. 171-206). Baltimore, MD: York Press.

3.

Celce-Murcia, M., Donna, B., & Goodwin, J. M. (2010). Teaching pronunciation: a course book and reference guide. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

4.

Chang, C., & Chang, C. K. (2014). Developing students' listening metacognitive strategies using online videotext self-dictation-generation learning activity. The EUROCALLReview, 22(1), 3-19.

5.

Choung, Y. A. (2014). Listening difficulties and problems in using listening strategies for EFL high school students. The Journal of Modern British and American Language and Literature, 32(4), 75-102.

6.

Desmerules-Trudel, F. (2018). Spoken word recognition in native and second language Canadian French: Phonetic detail and representation of vowel nasalization (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON.

7.

Educational Testing Service [ETS]. (2018). TOEIC Kichwul Kongsik Conghapse LC [TOEIC official test preparation book LC]. Seoul, Korea: YBM.

8.

Hagiwara, A., & Kuzumaki, Y. (1982). An analysis of errors in listening dictation with specific reference to the cause of misperception of English pronunciation. System, 10(1), 53-60.

9.

Hwang, M. H. (2004). Exploring listening comprehension difficulties observed among Korean high school students. The Journal of Curriculum & Evaluation, 7(1), 275-301.

10.

Jeong, Y., & Koo, N. (2017, December). A study of Korean EFL students’ listening difficulties in phonetics. Proceedings of the Mirae English Language and Literature Association Conference (pp. 160-164). Jeju, Korea.

11.

Johnson, K. (2004, January). Massive reduction in conversational American English. In K. Yoneyama & K. Maekawa (Eds.), Spontaneous speech: Data and analysis. Proceedings of the 1st session of the 10th International Symposium (pp. 29-54). Tokyo, Japan: The National International Institute for Japanese Language.

12.

Kang, S. M. (2017). Connected speech in listening and speaking. The Journal of Linguistic Science, 80(1), 1-15.

13.

Kim, W. B. (2002). Efficient listening teaching strategies through the analysis of english listening errors. The Modern English Education, 3(2), 3-22.

14.

Kotani, K. & Yoshimi, T. (2015, October-November). Design of a Learner Corpus for Listening and Speaking Performance. Proceedings of the 29th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation (pp. 351-358). Shanghai, China.

15.

Shin, S. H., & Hwang, Y. (2012). Perception and production of English geminate consonants across word boundaries by Korean learners and native speakers of English. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology, 18(1), 85-110.

16.

Shoemaker, E. M. (2010). The exploitation of fine phonetic detail in the processing of L2 French. In B. VanPatten & J. Jegerski (Eds.), Research in second language processing and parsing (pp. 259-280). New York, NY: John Benjamins.

17.

Rubin, J. (1994). A review of second language listening comprehension research. The Modern Language Journal, 78(2), 199-221.

18.

RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: integrated development environment for R. [Computer software]. Boston, MA: RStudio. Retrieved from http://www.rstudio.com/

19.

UCLES [University of Cambridge Local Examinations Syndicate]. (2021). Cambridge English Proficiency: Frequently Asked Questions. Retrieved from https://www.cambridgeenglish.org/images/248530-cambridge-english-proficiency-faqs.pdf

20.

Voss, B. (1984). Slips of the ear. Tubingen, Gremany: Gunter Narr Verlag.

21.

Yang, J. H., & Kang, M. (2020). Application of a self-annotated transcription task in an EFL listening context. Modern English Education, 21(3), 37-47.

22.

Zhang, Q., & Wang, L. (2019, August). Perception of American English junctures by Chinese EFL learners. Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia.